|
Affichage de 1 - 7 of 7 (Bibliographie: Bibliographie WIKINDX globale)
Parameters:
|
Category: Polysémie
|
|
Flieller, A., & Tournois, J. (1994). Imagery value, subjective and objective frequency, date of entry into the language, and degree of polysemy in a sample of 998 french words. International Journal of Psychology, 29(4), p. p471.
|
Ajoutée par: Lynda Taabane
|
|
Pop. 26.79%
|
|
Gernsbacher, M. A. (1984). Resolving 20 years of inconsistent interactions between lexical familiarity and orthography, concreteness, and polysemy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113(2), p. p256–281.
|
Ajoutée par: Lynda Taabane
|
|
Pop. 35.06%
|
|
Hino, Y., & Lupker, S. J. (1996). Effects of polysemy in lexical decision and naming: An alternative to lexical access accounts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22(6), p. p1331–1356.
|
Ajoutée par: Lynda Taabane
|
|
Pop. 31.61%
|
|
Klein, D. E., & Murphy, G. L. (2001). The representation of polysemous words. Journal of Memory and Language, 45(2), p. p259–282.
|
Ajoutée par: Lynda Taabane
|
|
Pop. 31.97%
|
|
Klein, D. E., & Murphy, G. L. (2002). Paper has been my ruin: Conceptual relations of polysemous senses. Journal of Memory and Language, 47(4), p. p548–570.
|
Ajoutée par: Lynda Taabane
|
|
Pop. 33.06%
|
|
Lichacz, F. M., Herdman, C. M., LeFevre, J.-A., & Baird, B. (1999). Polysemy effects in word naming. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53(2), p. p189–193.
|
Ajoutée par: Lynda Taabane
|
|
Pop. 31.24%
|
|
Pylkkänen, L., Llinás, R., & Murphy, G. L. (2006). The representation of polysemy: Meg evidence. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(1), p. p97–109.
|
Ajoutée par: Lynda Taabane
|
|
Pop. 28.88%
|
|
|